Closing the '30 million word gap' for children

Closing the '30 million word gap' for children

In 2006, Dr Dana Suskind, a paediatric surgeon at the University of Chicago, started performing delicate two-hour operations to attach a cochlear implant in the inner ears of babies born with hearing problems.

The implants enabled deaf and severely hearing-impaired children to hear electronically by capturing sound, translating it into digital signals and then electrical energy, and transmitting that to the auditory nerve.

But within a few years, she noticed that outcomes among her young patients varied.

Some of the children she operated on started to speak after the implant surgery, but others fell behind. What ate at her even more was that the children who tended to lag behind were the ones from poor, disadvantaged homes.

She signed up for a course on children's language development, and found her answer in a study by child psychologists Betty Hart and Todd Risley.

Their landmark study in the 1990s found that children whose parents were professionals were exposed to far more spoken words - more than 1,500 per hour, on average - than children from homes on welfare. Over one year, that amounted to a difference of nearly eight million words - by age four, that amounted to a total gap of 32 million words.

Moreover, it was not just the number of words, but also the type of words that varied. There was a substantial gap in the complexity of words being used.

The researchers also found that higher-income families provided their children with far more words of praise than low-income families did.

Children from families with professional backgrounds experienced six encouragements for every discouragement. Those from working-class families received two encouragements for every discouragement. The ratio was reversed for those from families on welfare, who heard two discouragements for every encouragement.

Twenty-nine of the 42 families in the original study were recruited for a follow-up study when the children were in third grade. Researchers found that measures of accomplishment at age three were highly indicative of performance at the ages of nine and 10. The foundation built at age three had an impact later on in life.

Moved by the findings, Dr Suskind started Project Aspire, an educational programme for parents of children with hearing loss. It taught parents how to provide the right language environment at home.

But she was mindful of the fact that the research did not point to a gap just for hearing-impaired children, but for all children born into families unable to provide for their healthy development.

"We look at these two-year-olds and already you can predict their life course. How can we, in all conscience, allow that?"

She launched the Thirty Million Words Initiative in 2009, with the goal of teaching parents and early-childhood caregivers how to nurture brain development through frequent, high-quality communication.

Her message is simple, but powerful.

"Children aren't born smart. They are made smart by their parents talking with them."

She adds: "Thirty Million Word Gap - it's like a headline for a news story. It grabs the attention of people. But behind it is this rich science that shows how the way parents interact with their children affects the way the children's brains develop.

"And it is really about helping parents understand the important role they can play."

The programme is delivered over a three-month period by trained staff who make weekly visits to the child's home.

The children's language development is tracked and fed back to their parents. The home visitors give each family - all of whom volunteered to participate - a tiny recording device, known as a Lena (short for Language Environment Analysis), which can be inserted into a vest worn by the child.

The recorders distinguish between words overheard from television or other electronics and words heard in live human conversations. Computer software then analyses the words spoken.

Explains Dr Suskind: "It is like a pedometer, but it keeps track of the words that children hear."

The strategies are simplified for the parents. They are told to follow three Ts - Tune In (to what the children say), Talk More (to the children using a rich vocabulary to build up the number of words heard) and Take Turns (to have a conversation with the children and actively listen to them).

Parents are taught how to incorporate rich, meaningful conversations into routine activities.

For example, while dressing their children, parents are encouraged to discuss the activity - which arm goes first while putting on a shirt, for example, or why hot weather makes shorts a good choice.

Instead of telling a child, "Put your shoes on", the parent might say instead, "It is time to go out. What do we have to do?"

"We teach them to add more language and interactions to things they're already doing, rather than adding more things to their already busy lives," says Mrs Beth Suskind, who is the project's co-director and Dr Suskind's sister-in-law.

Other topics covered include how to go on a television "diet", how to incorporate numbers into a conversation and techniques for reading to a child.

Throughout the 12-week programme, the Lena data is used to gauge progress and set new goals.

Small trials conducted by Dr Suskind's team have shown dramatic increases in parent-child interaction.

In one randomised control study involving 40 families, the experimental group received eight weeks of lessons on topics and strategies for integrating more words into daily life. During the intervention, in addition to speaking more with their kids, parents in this group significantly increased back-and-forth conversation.

Another study published in a journal two years ago showed how the programme could help children from affluent homes.

Dr Suskind and her team monitored 17 nannies, most of whom worked for University of Chicago faculty, who were caring for children aged 10 months to three years. After a single home visit focusing on language enrichment, the nannies used 395 more words per hour on average, a 32 per cent increase. They gave children a chance to respond 14 times more per hour, a 25 per cent increase.

Dr Suskind stresses that it is important to study if the programme really works - which is why a long-term study tracking between 200 and 250 children was launched early this year. They will be followed from the age of 15 months to five years, and their vocabulary development and school readiness will be monitored.

"You really need to be able to measure differences," Dr Suskind says. "Unless you can show that you are making it better, you are just running a programme to make yourself feel better."

She attempts to explain what drives her. Her mother is a social worker and her father, once a Peace Corps physician in Senegal, spent years as a paediatrics professor specialising in child malnutrition.

Her late husband was also an inspiration. Dr Donald Liu, who was surgeon-in-chief at Comer Children's Hospital, excelled at minimally invasive surgery procedures for children. He drowned three years ago trying to save two children who were caught in a strong current in Lake Michigan. The boys made it safely to shore but not Dr Liu.

"When he saw the two children struggle, there was no hesitation on his part. The children needed help, the children got help. He would never have considered standing on the shoreline while two children struggled, even if he had known it would cost him his life.

"In our country, we have too many children who are struggling against the odds of achievement. They are floundering. We cannot stand on the shoreline."

sandra@sph.com.sg


This article was first published on May 24, 2015.
Get a copy of The Straits Times or go to straitstimes.com for more stories.

This website is best viewed using the latest versions of web browsers.